Native
Language Governs The Way Toddlers Interpret Speech Sounds
Science Daily — Toddlers are learning language skills earlier than
expected and by the age of 18 months understand enough of the lexicon of
their own language to recognize how speakers use sounds to convey meaning.
They also ignore sounds that don't play a significant role in speaking their
native tongue, according to a study by a University of Pennsylvania
psychologist.
The study shows how important the child's first year is in acquiring
language. By listening to their parents and learning words, children
discover how speech in their language works, a process that is vital for
gaining command of vocabulary and grammar.
This is the first time scientists have shown that children as young as 18
months actively interpret the phonetic characteristics of their particular
language when they learn words. Previously, scientists had speculated that
this ability would emerge much later in life, once children had already
amassed large vocabularies.
Previous research showed that at birth infants can distinguish most of the
phonetic contrasts used by all the world's languages. This ''universal''
capacity shifts over the first year to a language-specific pattern in which
infants retain or improve categorization of native-language sounds but fail
to discriminate many non-native sounds. Eventually, they learn to ignore
subtle speech distinctions that their language does not use.
This is why Japanese toddlers, like Japanese adults, cannot tell apart the
English "r" and "l" sounds and why English speakers have trouble with
certain French vowels because they all sound the same to non-native speakers
due to language learning in infancy. The Penn study shows that even when two
words sound very different, toddlers know whether to take this difference
seriously or to chalk it up to random variation depending on how their
language works.
"The results demonstrate that at 18 months children have a rudimentary
understanding of the 'sound system' of their language and that knowledge
guides their interpretation of the sounds they encounter," said Daniel
Swingley, assistant professor in the Department of Psychology at Penn who
worked with colleagues from the University of British Columbia and the
Max-Planck-Institute for Psycholinguistics.
"Children can easily hear how the same word can be pronounced in different
ways. We might say, 'Is that your kiiiiiitty"' or, 'Show me the kitty.' In
English, we're still talking about the same cat. But children have to figure
this out. In other languages, like Japanese or Finnish, those two versions
of "kitty" could mean completely different things. Our study showed that
18-month-olds have already learned this and apply that knowledge when
learning new words."
Psychologists tested vowel duration ("kitty" versus "kiiiitty") in three
experiments comparing Dutch- and English-learning 18-month-olds. Children
were shown two different toys. With one toy, researchers repeated a word
dozens of times, naming it a "tam." The other toy was named too, with the
same label only with the vowel acoustically longer in duration ("taam").
Dutch children, learning a language that includes words differentiated by
how long the vowel is pronounced, interpret the variations as meaningful and
learn which word goes with each object. English speakers ignored the
elongation of vowel sounds.
English learners did not somehow lack the cognitive power to learn both
words. They can hear the difference between the words, and they succeed on
words that really are different in English ("tam" vs. "tem"). The difference
arose from the phonological generalizations children had already made from
their brief experience with English: "tam" and "taam", like "kitty" and "kiiiitty",
mean the same thing. Dutch children, on the other hand, interpreted vowel
duration as lexically contrastive in keeping with the properties of their
language.
The study, to appear in the Oct. 1 issue of the Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, was funded by the Max-Planck-Gesellschaft, the
Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek's Spinoza Prize, the
National Science Foundation, the National Institutes of Health and the
Canadian Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council.
The study was performed by Swingley, Christiane Dietrich of the
Max-Planck-Institute for Psycholinguistics and Janet F. Werker of the
University of British Columbia.
Note: This story has been adapted from material provided by University
of Pennsylvania
|
|